Utilisateur
Basic emotions are experienced by all humans and have distinct features.
Guilt and shame are examples of complex emotions.
Basic emotions include happiness, anger, and fear.
Components of emotions develop independently with different developmental trajectories.
The outcome of self-organization in a dynamic system is called an attractor state.
Self-organization in emotions is context-dependent.
Congenitally blind people produce similar spontaneous emotional facial expressions as sighted people.
Ekman & Friesen's study in New Guinea found that isolated tribes could recognize emotions in pictures similar to Westerners.
Infants can discriminate between facial emotional expressions at 5 months old.
Five-month-old infants can discriminate between sadness and disgust.
Infants can discriminate between happiness and surprise.
Five-month-old infants fail to show sensitivity to the categorical boundary between anger and disgust.
Infants can distinguish between sadness and anger.
Attention focus involves shifting attention away from an upsetting event.
Older participants report less use of suppression over time.
Adolescents use a variety of emotion regulation strategies, including adaptive and maladaptive strategies.
Infants use caregivers' emotional signals to guide their behavior in uncertain situations.
The Test of Emotion Comprehension (TEC) supports the understanding of emotion in children aged 3 to 10 years.
Emotional comprehension involves recognizing, naming, and understanding external causes of emotions.
Empathic processes are automatically elicited but shaped by top-down control processes.
Attachment is related to the development of empathy.
Attention focus involves shifting attention away from an upsetting event.
Older participants report less use of suppression over time.
Adolescents use a variety of emotion regulation strategies, including adaptive and maladaptive strategies.
There is evidence of emotion-specific use of regulation strategies.
Expressive suppression increases for sadness and fear but not for anger during adolescence.
Adolescents at age 15 report less use of emotion regulation strategies compared to other age groups.
Infants showed a variety of facial expressions in response to different situations.
Joy expressions were the most common in response to tickling.
Surprise expressions were most common in response to the jack-in-the-box.
Recognition of low-intensity emotional expressions follows a slower developmental trajectory.
Vocal emotion recognition continues to develop into late childhood.
Children's ability to recognize emotions improves steadily from 3 to 11 years old.
There is evidence of emotion-specific use of regulation strategies.
Expressive suppression increases for sadness and fear but not for anger during adolescence.
Adolescents at age 15 report less use of emotion regulation strategies compared to other age groups.
Understanding of situational causes of emotions is developed by age 5.
Understanding that emotions are connected to desires and beliefs develops around age 7.
The ability to recognize conflicting emotions develops by age 9.
Significant improvement in emotional comprehension is seen in all components between ages 3 and 11.
Fetuses produce a variety of facial expressions, including smiles and pain expressions, during non-painful ultrasounds.
Findings for prenatal facial expressions align with the dynamic systems view of emotional development.
The production of facial expressions in fetuses suggests an innate basis for emotional expression.
Congenitally blind people produce spontaneous emotional facial expressions similar to those of sighted people.
The lack of visual experience affects the ability to produce voluntary emotional expressions.
Congenitally blind people’s spontaneous expressions suggest that some aspects of emotional expression are innate.
A child learns to manage fear through repeated exposure to non-threatening situations, leading to a more flexible emotional response over time.
Two siblings develop different emotional responses to the same situation due to their unique experiences and interactions with their environment.
They would recognize basic emotions such as happiness, anger, and fear.
They might have difficulty distinguishing between complex emotions like guilt or shame.
Show the infant pictures of different facial expressions while naming the emotions.
Expose the infant to varied social interactions to observe different emotional expressions.
Regularly engage in activities that produce clear and distinct emotional reactions, such as playing peekaboo or using a jack-in-the-box.
Use toys that morph facial expressions to highlight differences in emotions.
Use positive vocal cues alone to reassure the child.
Use a combination of facial and vocal cues to guide the child's behavior.
Provide consistent and clear emotional signals to help the child understand the situation.
Encouraging children to discuss and share their feelings and experiences.
Modeling empathetic behavior in everyday interactions.
Providing opportunities for children to care for pets or younger siblings.
Using stories and role-playing exercises to explore different perspectives and emotions.
Teaching problem-solving skills and encouraging their use in real-life situations.
Encouraging the use of humor to reframe stressful events.
Providing support for cognitive reappraisal to reinterpret challenging situations.
Use age-appropriate discussions about how external events influence emotions.
Encourage children to talk about their desires and how they relate to their feelings.
Teach children to recognize and label their emotions accurately.
Provide scenarios where children can practice understanding mixed emotions.
Emphasizing the importance of universal expressions like happiness and sadness in communication.
Recognizing that voluntary emotional expressions might be misunderstood across cultures.
Being aware that spontaneous emotional expressions are more likely to be understood universally.
Using both facial and vocal cues to enhance emotional communication across cultures.
Encouraging the expression of emotions rather than suppression.
Teaching adolescents to seek social support when feeling sad or afraid.
Using mindfulness practices to help adolescents manage their emotional responses.
Helping adolescents understand and articulate their emotions through journaling.
Infants respond most commonly with surprise to a masked stranger, a Jack-in-the-box and arm restraint.
Discrete Emotion Perspective.
Infants most commonly respond with surprise to a Jack-in-the-box, anger to arm restraint, and fear to a masked stranger.
5-month-old infants can discriminate between different emotions of the same valence.